Autism and memory
The characteristics of memory function in autism has been studied for decades.[1] Individuals with autism have both specific difficulties with memory and memory strengths.
Memory difficulty is not part of the diagnostic criteria for autism spectrum disorder (ASD); but, it is a common difficulty experienced by many individuals with this diagnosis.[2] This article emphasizes declarative and non-declarative memory functions and their connection with autism. Habbi, Harris, Pollick and, Melville (2019) in their meta-analysis reported a connection between autism and working memory. Working memory impairments appear to involve both phonological and visual spatial arenas.[3]
Among areas of strength are certain types of visual memory[1] and intact rote memory, with some individuals showing very good rote memory.[4]
Overview
Autism is a spectrum disorder which presents many challenges to the study of autism and memory function. It is important for researchers to be able to collect data across the spectrum and develop experimental designs to study both individuals with high functioning autism (HFA) and those with moderate-low functioning autism (M-LFA).[5] Methods to classify individuals into one of these groups include intelligence testing, which enables researchers to make methodical comparisons and analyses between HFA and M-LFA individuals.[5] Alternatively, in recent years, “mixed” studies of both HFA and M-LFA participants are done as well.[5]
Some of the earliest references to the topic of autism and memory date back to the 1960s and 70s when several studies appeared proposing that autism should be classified as an amnesic disorder.[4] What is now diagnosed as autism, was formerly diagnosed as developmental amnesia.[5] Subsequent studies had mixed results; some found support for impaired memory, others found evidence of relatively intact memory function.[4] Even though the view of autism as an amnesic disorder of memory has now been rejected,[6] there are still many studies done on the deficits in memory function that are associated with autism.[6] Furthermore, in the memory studies there were observations of memory dysfunctions in autism that were consistent with a deficit in executive functions.[4] Theories have been developed stating that the memory deficits are a reflection of primary deficits in executive function, or of a primary deficit in the processing of complex information.[1]
It has been difficult to get consistent results in autism research. One of the reasons for this is that the autistic population is very diverse, with differences in development and cognitive levels.[1] However, there are a couple of findings that have been replicated in several studies:
- Children with autism do not use organizational strategies or context to support memory. For example, when remembering words, children with autism demonstrate the same level when they are in a random group and when they are semantically or syntactically organized.[1][7]
- Compared to other children, those with autism use fewer strategies spontaneously in order to retrieve information.[1]
- Children with autism have much more difficulties with memory for complex material - for instance, for young children, recall of digits was easiest, of sentences more difficult, and of stories the most difficult.[1]
- Visual memory for some types of material is an area of strength, but children with autism also show certain difficulties with complex material.[1]
Declarative memory
Declarative memory is memory which can be consciously recalled, such as facts and knowledge,.[8] For instance, autobiographical memory is an example of declarative memory. Declarative memory includes semantic and episodic memory. Semantic memory involves the recollection of facts, and episodic memory involves the recollection of previous experiences in life.[9] Studies on individuals with ASD have shown impairments to their episodic memory but a relative preservation of their semantic memory.[9] The brain regions that play a major role in declarative learning and memory are the hippocampus and regions of the medial temporal lobe.[9] Little research has been conducted on M-LFA individuals and memory function. Memory profiles of M-LFA individuals and of HFA individuals seem to be similar, but there is some evidence that declarative memory impairment is consistently greater for M-LFA individuals than for HFA individuals.[5]
Autobiographical memory
One aspect of autobiographical memory is the self-reference effect, which means that typically people have a stronger memory for information that is relevant to themselves.[10] It has been theorized that individuals with ASD have diminished psychological self-knowledge, but an intact physical self-knowledge. As a result, these individuals show impaired autobiographical episodic memory and a reduced self-reference effect (which may each rely on psychological aspects of the self-concept) but do not show specific impairments in memory for their own rather than others’ actions (which may rely on physical aspects of the self-concept).[10]
Impaired memory binding may also play a role, meaning individuals with autism have a hard time remembering events where they were involved.[11][12] Individuals with ASD are able to encode events at perceptual and objective levels, but they are unable to encode events in a subjective manner.[11]
A 2012 study conducted by Laura Crane et al., like previous studies, suggests that adults with ASD experienced a reduction in the speed of recall of the autobiographical memory as well as the detail of memory in comparison to typical adults.[2] However, the study results suggest that when adults with ASD were able to retrieve these memories their accounts were qualitatively similar to typical adults.[2]
Prospective memory
The landmark study conducted by Jones et al. in 2010[13] was the first to examine evidence-based prospective memory impairment for ASD individuals. The primary findings of this study provide evidence suggesting that poor social and communication skills in ASD individuals have an effect on their prospective memory function.
With these demonstrated connections between social communication skills and prospective memory, several hypotheses have arisen concerning what specific aspects of diminished social skills are influencing prospective memory impairment. Prospective memory relies heavily upon social motivation—like the desire to be socially included, insight and perception into other people's emotions, and the ability to read social cues. Individuals with ASD are largely tuned out to these types of social motivation, which then affects their prospective memory.[13] Difficulty reflecting upon one's own mental state and the mental state of others, self-awareness and self-monitoring deficiencies likely also play a role in impaired prospective memory.[13]
A recent study conducted by Altgassen et al. in 2012 also suggested that even though there are differences between individuals with ASD and typical adults when it comes to planning performance and organizational skills, there is no significant difference between individuals with ASD and typical participants when it comes to rule adherence.[14] In fact, ASD individuals tend to follow rules very closely once they have been learned. These findings suggest that everyday organizational task rules are not always evident in ASD people, and if they were targeted as rules it would be easier for them to follow.[14] Prospective memory in autism has not been widely studied, so further research needs to be conducted in this field in order to investigate if there is supporting evidence to this claim.
Cued recall
Cued recall, research has been conducted primarily with HFA individuals, but some studies have shown common weakness on cued recall tests for both groups of HFA and M-LFA individuals.[5] In another study, testing a HFA group and a control group of non-ASD individuals revealed that the ASD group made more intrusion errors on recall trials. This evidence also supports the hypothesis that ASD individuals may have a deficit in cued recall. Studies about temporal order memory and ASD participants also indicate that temporal order memory for verbal information is impaired in ASD individuals. However, there is also contradictory evidence for these claims.[4] A 2001 study conducted by Mottron et al. found that ASD individuals, unlike typical individuals, do not benefit more from semantic rather than phonological cues.[15] ASD individuals do not always conform to the Levels of Processing principle, since they benefit the same from deep and superficial recall cues.
In a recency judgment task study conducted by Gras-Vincendon et al.,[16] evidence suggested that temporal context memory is not impaired in individuals with HFA. The study proposes this is due largely because the recency judgment task involves more automatic than organizational processing. The researchers proposed that the supposedly impaired temporal order memory for verbal information found in Bennetto's[4] work is actually due to difficulty with general complex verbal stimuli.[16]
Recognition
Recognition in HFA individuals has been widely studied. Overall, these studies conclude that the majority of HFA individuals have intact recognition ability.[5] Non-social stimuli recognition is often superior, or “robustly intact”,[5] although there is some evidence suggesting that HFA individuals have difficulty with complex scenes and color combinations. For example, HFA individuals exhibit intact recognition of non-social stimuli such as written words, spoken sentences, pictures of common objects, and meaningless patterns or shapes. For HFA individuals impaired recognition have been found in object-location and object-color recognition tests, and recognition of words encoded self-referentially.[5] For more information regarding recognition of social stimuli for individuals with autism, see the face perception page.
Contrary to the plethora of HFA recognition memory studies, the study of recognition for M-LFA individuals is considerably lacking.[5] The studies that do exist predominantly point to impaired recognition of pictures, words to name objects and other non-social stimuli. Four delayed recognition studies reported recognition impairment for M-LFA participants.[5] Additionally, four of the seven primary studies of non-social stimuli recognition revealed significant impairment of non-social stimuli for M-LFA individuals.[5] The other three studies were less reliable because of methodology.[5] Boucher, Lewis and Collis gathered data supporting poor facial recognition, something widely observed for M-LFA individuals.[5]
Implicit/Non-declarative memory
Implicit memory is non-declarative memory that relies on past experiences to help recall things without actively thinking of them. Procedural memory, classical conditioning and priming are all included in implicit memory. For example, procedural skills, such as riding a bike, become so natural over time that one does not have to explicitly think about them. The brain regions that process implicit memory are the basal ganglia and the cerebellum. Research suggests that both HFA and M-LFA individuals show strong implicit memory function.[5] HFA individuals display intact implicit memory for non-social stimuli, unimpaired classical conditioning and performance on other implicit learning tasks. HFA individuals displayed normal perceptual and conceptual priming. Studies concerning implicit memory in M-LFA individuals are sparse and further study is needed.
As mentioned above, there are very few reliable studies of non-declarative memory for M-LFA. However, there are some speculations. Some consider that the impaired motor skills evident in many cases of M-LFA may suggest impaired procedural learning. Other studies, including Walenski (2006) and Romero-Muguia (2008),[5] also think that ASD behavior is indicative of implicit memory function. For example, many ASD individuals have exceptional implicit perceptual processing abilities in mathematics, the arts and in musical improvisation.[5] Furthermore, there is speculation that because M-LFA individuals are often characterized by habit and routine, habit formation is likely unimpaired.[5] Gordon et al. (2007) studied how procedural learning is affected by autism and found that even M-LFA individuals are capable of learning an implicit sequence with the help of behavioral training.[17] Behavioral treatments and therapies used with M-LFA individuals are usually very helpful, suggesting that implicit knowledge can be acquired and conditioning is intact.[5]
Behavioral treatments and therapies used with M-LFA individuals are usually very helpful, suggesting that implicit knowledge can be acquired and conditioning is intact.[5]
Memory strengths
Although many people who have been diagnosed with autism suffer some memory difficulties, there are some who excel with memory. There are some individuals with HFA who have been diagnosed with Savant syndrome, such as Kim Peek.[18] Those who are considered savants have abilities, usually related to memory, that are far above average, while also experiencing mental disabilities. Savants can also excel in a range of skills other than memory, including math, art and music. While those who are diagnosed with autism have a higher probability of having savant skills, men are also more likely that women to exhibit these skills.[19][20] Howlin et al. (2009) studied a group of 137 individuals with autism, ranging from HFA to M-LFA, and found that 28.5% of them had savant skills.[19]
Another well-known savant is Daniel Tammet, who gave a Ted talk on his experience with synesthesia.[21] Synesthesia is a condition that causes the senses to be perceived differently. Those who have it may experience colors, tastes, sounds and shapes while perceiving numbers, letters and people’s faces or names. It is more common for individuals with autism to have synesthesia or savant syndrome, compared to the rest of the population.[22]
Neurological
Studies suggest that memory deficit observed in ASD has a biological explanation linked to abnormalities in the hippocampus and other neural regions responsible for strategic regulation, such as the amygdala and the frontal cortex.[11]
Further research
Across the board, it is agreed that there is an urgent need for more research concerning M-LFA memory function. Specifically, there are about half as many M-LFA studies on memory compared to HFA.[5] Very few studies of implicit/non-declarative memory exist and almost no brain studies. Almost all of the M-LFA studies are conducted with school-aged children, so there is a great need for a wider age range in memory studies.[5] Very little mapping of the developmental process of ASD has happened, so there is an overall need for longitudinal studies of individuals with ASD across their lifetime.[13] Replication and extension of prospective memory research is needed[13] as well as further cross-sectional and comparison studies for HFA and M-LFA. Recently a specific call has been issued for the investigation of total loss of declarative memory in significantly low-functioning, nonverbal individuals with ASD.[5]
References
- Diane L. Williams; Gerald Goldstein; Nancy J. Minshew (January 2006). "The Profile of Memory Function in Children With Autism". Neuropsychology. 20 (1): 21–29. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.20.1.21. PMC 1847594. PMID 16460219.
- Crane, L., Pring, L., Jukes, K., & Goddard, L. (2012). Patterns of autobiographical memory in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2100–2112. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1459-2
- Habib, Abdullah; Harris, Leanne; Pollick, Frank; Melville, Craig (2019). "A meta-analysis of working memory in individuals with autism spectrum disorders". PLOS ONE. 14 (4): e0216198. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0216198. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 6490940. PMID 31039192.
- Loisa Bennetto; Brnee F. Pennington; Sally J. Rogers (1996). "Intact and Impaired Memory Functions in Autism" (PDF). Child Development. 67 (4): 1816–1835. doi:10.2307/1131734. JSTOR 1131734. PMID 8890510.
- Boucher J.; Mayes A.; Bigham S. (2012). "Memory in autistic spectrum disorder" (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 138 (3): 458–496. doi:10.1037/a0026869. PMID 22409507.
- Williams DL, Goldstein G, Minshew NJ (August 2006). "Neuropsychologic functioning in children with autism: further evidence for disordered complex information-processing". Child Neuropsychol. 12 (4–5): 279–298. doi:10.1080/09297040600681190. PMC 1803025. PMID 16911973.
- Renner, Peggy; Klinger, Laura Grofer; Klinger, Mark R. (2000-02-01). "Implicit and Explicit Memory in Autism: Is Autism an Amnesic Disorder?". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 30 (1): 3–14. doi:10.1023/A:1005487009889. ISSN 1573-3432. PMID 10819116. S2CID 44545479.
- Ullman MT. Contributions of memory circuits to language: the declarative/procedural model" Cognition 2004; 92: 231–70.
- Suzanne Goh; Bradley S. Peterson (2012). "Imaging evidence for disturbances in multiple learning and memory systems in persons with autism spectrum disorders". Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 54 (3): 208–213. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.04153.x. PMID 22269006. S2CID 35999361.
- Lind S (2010). "Memory and the self in Autism: A review and theoretical framework" (PDF). Autism. 14 (5): 430–455. doi:10.1177/1362361309358700. PMID 20671017. S2CID 12452530.
- Wojcik, D. Z., Moulin, C. A., & Souchay, C. (2013). Metamemory in children with autism: Exploring "feeling-of-knowing" in episodic and semantic memory. Neuropsychology, 19-27. doi:10.1037/a0030526
- Millward, Claire; Powell, Stuart; Messer, David; Jordan, Rita (2000-02-01). "Recall for Self and Other in Autism: Children's Memory for Events Experienced by Themselves and Their Peers". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 30 (1): 15–28. doi:10.1023/A:1005455926727. ISSN 1573-3432. PMID 10819117. S2CID 9298592.
- Jones C. G.; Happé F.; Pickles A.; Marsden A. S.; Tregay J.; Baird G.; Charman T. (2011). "'Everyday memory' impairments in autism spectrum disorders". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 41 (4): 455–464. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.690.1877. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-1067-y. PMID 20635196. S2CID 8789194.
- Altgassen, M., Koban, N., & Kliegel, M. (2012). Do adults with autism spectrum disorders compensate in naturalistic prospective memory tasks?. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2141–2151. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1466-3
- Mottron, L., Morasse, K., & Belleville, S. (2001). A study of memory functioning in individuals with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 253-260. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00716
- Gras-Vincendon A.; Mottron L.; Salame P.; Bursztejn C.; Danion J. (2007). "Temporal context memory in high-functioning autism". Autism. 11 (6): 523–534. doi:10.1177/1362361307083257. PMID 17947288. S2CID 33511585.
- Gordon, Barry; Stark, Shauna (February 2007). "Procedural Learning of a Visual Sequence in Individuals With Autism". Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities. 22 (1): 14–22. doi:10.1177/10883576070220010201. ISSN 1088-3576. S2CID 145287722.
- Chi, Richard P.; Snyder, Allan W. (May 2012). "Brain stimulation enables the solution of an inherently difficult problem". Neuroscience Letters. 515 (2): 121–124. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2012.03.012. ISSN 0304-3940. PMID 22440856. S2CID 17008847.
- Howlin, Patricia; Goode, Susan; Hutton, Jane; Rutter, Michael (2009-05-27). "Savant skills in autism: psychometric approaches and parental reports". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 364 (1522): 1359–1367. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0328. ISSN 0962-8436. PMC 2677586. PMID 19528018.
- Ruthsatz, Joanne; Petrill, Stephen A.; Li, Ning; Wolock, Samuel L.; Bartlett, Christopher W. (2015). "Molecular Genetic Evidence for Shared Etiology of Autism and Prodigy". Human Heredity. 79 (2): 53–59. doi:10.1159/000373890. ISSN 0001-5652. PMID 25791271. S2CID 31954593.
- "Daniel Tammet: Different ways of knowing". 2011. doi:10.1037/e580542011-001. Cite journal requires
|journal=
(help) - Bouvet, Lucie; Donnadieu, Sophie; Valdois, Sylviane; Caron, Chantal; Dawson, Michelle; Mottron, Laurent (2014). "Veridical mapping in savant abilities, absolute pitch, and synesthesia: an autism case study". Frontiers in Psychology. 5: 106. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00106. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 3927080. PMID 24600416.